Haven't signed into your Scholastic account before?
Teachers, not yet a subscriber?
Subscribers receive access to the website and print magazine.
You are being redirecting to Scholastic's authentication page...
Announcements & Tutorials
Renew Now, Pay Later
Sharing Google Activities
2 min.
Setting Up Student View
Exploring Your Issue
Using Text to Speech
Join Our Facebook Group!
1 min.
Subscriber Only Resources
Access this article and hundreds more like it with a subscription to Science World magazine.
STANDARDS
NGSS: Core Idea: LS1.D
CCSS: Reading Informational Text: 1
TEKS: 6.2D, 7.2D, 8.2D, B.2D, B.4A
Article Options
Presentation View
Lexile® measure
Investigate It!
Feline Friends
A scientist tests whether cats know the names of their fellow felines
AS YOU READ, THINK ABOUT how scientists can study animals’ understanding of the world around them.
SHUTTERSTOCK.COM
COZY COMPANIONS: These cats live together, but do they know each other’s names?
Cats often maintain an independent streak, even in the company of their favorite human family members. They decide for themselves whether to come when called, curl up in a person’s lap, or accept an affectionate scratch on the chin. Sometimes, they may even act as if they’re ignoring the people around them—for example, a cat might appear not to notice when their owner speaks to them. But a new study suggests that cats may pay closer attention to people’s speech than they let on.
Cats often have an independent streak, even around their favorite human family members. When cats are called, will they come? Will they curl up in a person’s lap or accept a loving scratch on the chin? Cats decide that for themselves. Sometimes, they even seem to ignore the people around them. For example, a cat’s owner might speak to it, and the cat might appear not to notice. But cats may pay closer attention to people’s speech than they let on. That’s what a new study suggests.
SAHO TAKAGI
Saho Takagi
Saho Takagi is a cognitive scientist at Azabu University in Japan. She studies how animals think and behave. She also lives with two cats. Not long ago, Takagi was speaking with another cat owner who mentioned an interesting interaction they’d witnessed between their pets. The owner had called one of their cats by name. The cat didn’t react. But a second kitty in the room turned and looked at the cat the owner had addressed. It was as if the other feline had recognized its friend’s name.
That observation got Takagi wondering: Do cats who live together learn each other’s names? Or was the second cat’s behavior a coincidence? Takagi and a group of fellow scientists designed an investigation to find out.
Saho Takagi is a cognitive scientist at Azabu University in Japan. She studies how animals think and behave. She also lives with two cats. Not long ago, Takagi was speaking with another cat owner. The person had witnessed an interesting interaction between their pets. The owner had called one of their cats by name. The cat didn’t react. But a second kitty in the room turned and looked at the first cat. The second cat had seemed to recognize its friend’s name.
That made Takagi wonder: Do cats who live together learn each other’s names? Or did the second cat just happen to look at the first cat? Takagi and some fellow scientists designed a study to find out.
TESTING WHAT CATS KNOW
Determining whether a person knows someone’s name is easy: Just ask! But since cats can’t communicate with words, the team had to come up with a different way to test the animals’ knowledge. “We used a technique called expectancy violation, which is often used with human infants,” says Takagi. “It describes animals’ tendency to take a longer look at an event when it differs from their expectations.” In other words, when something surprising happens, an animal will stare at it longer than at an everyday occurrence
It’s easy to find out if a person knows someone’s name. Just ask! But cats can’t communicate with words. So the team had to find a different way to test the animals’ knowledge. “We used a technique called expectancy violation, which is often used with human infants,” says Takagi. “It describes animals’ tendency to take a longer look at an event when it differs from their expectations.” In other words, animals will stare longer at something surprising.
WHO’S THAT? Scientists showed cats photos of their feline roommates on a laptop.
Takagi’s team recruited 48 cats for their experiment. Of these felines, 19 lived in households with other cats. The rest were residents of cat cafés—establishments popular in Japan—where customers can interact with a whole group of kitties who are sometimes available for adoption. The researchers wanted to compare cats from these environments because “we thought there might be a difference in the number of times a cat is called by name at a home compared to at a cat café,” says Takagi. Café cats spend a lot of time with visitors who might not know them well, so these cats may have fewer opportunities to hear their companions’ names.
The scientists recorded the voices of owners and café caretakers calling out their cats’ names. They also took photos of each cat’s face. For the experiment, a researcher gently held a feline subject, or study participant, in place in front of a laptop. The scientists played an audio recording of the owner or caretaker calling the name of another cat who lives with the subject. Then a photo of a cat’s face appeared on the screen for seven seconds. Sometimes the photo matched the name the subject had just heard, and sometimes it didn’t. The researchers timed how long each cat looked at the photo (see Names and Faces).
Takagi’s team chose 48 cats for their experiment. Nineteen of them lived in households with other cats. The rest lived in cat cafés, which are popular in Japan. In these cafés, customers can interact with a group of kitties. Sometimes these cats are up for adoption. The researchers wanted to compare cats from these environments. “We thought there might be a difference in the number of times a cat is called by name at a home compared to a cat café,” says Takagi. Café cats spend a lot of time with visitors who might not know them well. So these cats may not hear their friends’ names as often.
Owners and café caretakers called out their cats’ names. The scientists recorded their voices. The scientists also took photos of each cat’s face. For the experiment, a researcher gently held a cat in place in front of a laptop. The scientists played an audio recording for this feline subject, or study participant. It was the owner or caretaker calling the name of another cat from the subject’s home. Then a photo of a cat’s face appeared on the screen for seven seconds. Sometimes the photo matched the name in the recording. Sometimes it didn’t. The researchers timed how long each subject looked at the photo (see Names and Faces).
In the experiment, cats who live in a family household stared at a screen longer when shown the picture of a cat that didn’t match the name they’d just heard.
LEONIEK VAN DER VLIET/EYEEM/GETTY IMAGES (LEFT CAT); ANNI SOMMER/IMAGEBROKER RF/GETTY IMAGES (RIGHT CAT)
(left) MATCHED: Subject hears: "Yuki", Subject sees: Yuki; Reaction: Subject looks at photo briefly. (right) MISMATCHED: Subject hears: "Yuki", Subject sees: Muffin; Reaction: Subject stares at photo longer.
EXPECTING A FRIEND
Analyzing the data, Takagi’s team found that café cats spent similar amounts of time looking at the screen regardless of whether the name and face matched. That suggests they didn’t know their fellow café residents’ names and so weren’t surprised by the mismatch.
Household cats, on the other hand, tended to stare at the screen for a longer time when the photo displayed didn’t match the name they’d just heard. Their response suggests that these kitty subjects were confused by the mismatch between name and face: When they hear the name of a cat who lives with them, that’s who they expect to see. That implies that household cats know one another’s names.
Takagi says the study’s results show that we shouldn’t underestimate feline smarts. “Cats understand a lot more than we can imagine.”
Takagi’s team studied the data. They found that café cats didn’t seem to notice whether the name and face matched. These cats spent similar amounts of time looking at the screen either way. They didn’t seem surprised by a mismatch. That suggests they didn’t know their fellow café cats’ names.
The results were different with household cats. When the photo on the screen didn’t match the name in the recording, they tended to stare at the screen longer. The mismatch between name and face seemed to confuse them. When they hear the name of a cat who that lives with them, they expect to see that cat. That suggests that household cats know one another’s names.
Takagi says we can learn something from the study: Cats may be smarter than you think! “Cats understand a lot more than we can imagine.”
Scientists design experiments to try to answer research questions. Consider the investigation of cats conducted by Saho Takagi and her team. Then answer the following questions.
What question did Takagi want to answer?
What’s one challenge the researchers encountered in determining whether a cat knows another cat’s name?
Explain the test the scientists devised to examine cats’ knowledge.
Why do you think household cats and café cats showed different behaviors in the experiment?